Abstract
Over the past few years, genome-wide association studies have identified many trait-associated loci that have different effects on females and males, which increased attention to the genetic architecture differences between the sexes. The between-sex differences in genetic architectures can cause a variety of phenomena such as differences in the effect sizes at trait-associated loci, differences in the magnitudes of polygenic background effects, and differences in the phenotypic variances. However, current association testing approaches for dealing with sex, such as including sex as a covariate, cannot fully account for these phenomena and can be suboptimal in statistical power. We present a novel association mapping framework, MetaSex, that can comprehensively account for the genetic architecture differences between the sexes. Through simulations and applications to real data, we show that our framework has superior performance than previous approaches in association mapping.
GENOME-WIDE association studies (GWAS) have successfully identified numerous genetic loci associated with complex human traits. In recent years, increasing attention has been paid to the sex difference in genetic architectures in GWAS. A number of studies have found differences in effect sizes between males and females on loci associated with traits (Magi et al. 2010; Boraska et al. 2012; Fox et al. 2012; Kostis et al. 2012; Mason and Lehert 2012; Chen et al. 2013; Kubo et al. 2013; Peters et al. 2013; Porcu et al. 2013; Randall et al. 2013; Kang et al. 2014; Ohmen et al. 2014). In particular, a meta-analysis of 46 studies of anthropomorphic phenotypes discovered seven loci with different effects between the sexes (Randall et al. 2013). Recently, Winkler et al. performed a meta-analysis of 114 studies on the waist/hip ratio adjusted for body mass index (BMI) to discover 44 loci showing significant sex-specific effects. Of these, 11 loci showed opposite effects between the sexes (Winkler et al. 2015).
It remains unclear how best to account for the sex difference in genetic architectures in association mapping. One traditional approach is to analyze each sex separately using sex-specific tests (SSTs). This approach is optimal for detecting sex-specific effects that only exist in one sex, but is not powerful for detecting effects that exist in both sexes. Another traditional approach is to analyze the whole sample and use sex as a covariate (CV). This approach is optimal for detecting effects that exist in both sexes in a constant effect size, but is not powerful for detecting sex-interacting effects that exist in both sexes in differing effect sizes.
In the present study, we first enumerate three possible phenomena that can be caused by the sex difference in genetic architectures. One is the effect size difference between the sexes at the associated locus, which was observed in previous studies (Randall et al. 2013; Winkler et al. 2015). Another is the effect size difference between the sexes at numerous loci spread throughout the genome with small effects, which can be manifested as the polygenic background effects that interact with sex. The final one is the phenotypic variance difference between the sexes, which can be caused by many factors such as sex acting as a biological environment (e.g., hormone difference) and sex interacting with external environments (e.g., lifestyle difference). We show that these phenomena can often be observed in human traits collected in the North Finland Birth Cohort (NFBC) dataset (Sabatti et al. 2009).
Here, we present a novel association mapping framework, MetaSex, that can account for the potential sex difference in genetic architectures. Our framework comprehensively deals with the three aforementioned phenomena by uniquely combining linear mixed model and meta-analysis. Our linear mixed model includes five variance components, where three components capture sex-interacting polygenic effects and two components capture sex-interacting variances. We then combine the observed effect sizes of the two sexes using the random effects model meta-analysis (RE) (Han and Eskin 2011) that provides high power for detecting sex-interacting effects. This whole procedure can be computationally challenging because the five variance component model is impractically slow to apply to millions of markers in GWAS. Therefore, we propose an approximated model that splits the five variance component model into two sex-specific models, each including only two variance components. Using simulations and real data, we demonstrate that our framework can powerfully detect associations in a wide range of situations.
Materials and Methods
MetaSex
Overview:
We first provide an overview of our proposed framework. We constructed a toy example with six individuals (three females and three males). The equation in Figure 1A shows the components in our model for testing a single SNP. In this equation, vector y is the observed phenotype measurements, where subscripts and
denote females and males. μ denotes the phenotypic mean. h is the sex status indicator (female = 1 and male = 0), which is included as a CV to account for the sex-specific phenotypic mean. The first column of X is the genotype vector of the SNP, whose effect size is β. The second column of X is the genotype-by-sex interaction term (SNP × h), whose effect size is
is a variance component that models the polygenic background effects from the genome-wide loci that affect both sexes. Consistent with the standard linear mixed model (Kang et al. 2008, 2010; Zhou and Stephens 2012), we assume that
follows a normal distribution with mean zero and variance-covariance matrix
, where K is the kinship matrix representing the relationship between individuals.
is an additional variance component that we introduce, which represents the female-specific polygenic effects. We assume that
has mean zero and variance
where ∘ indicates element-wise multiplication. Similarly,
is a variance component representing the male-specific polygenic effects, which has mean zero and variance
We then model separate error terms for females and males, assuming that error variances can be different.
is a female-specific error term that follows a normal distribution with mean zero and variance
where I is an identity matrix. Similarly,
is a male-specific error term that has mean zero and variance
Overview of the MetaSex framework. (A) A linear mixed model with five variance components (full model). (B) Decomposition of the full model into two sex-specific linear mixed models with two variance components (approximated model). (C) Given the sex-specific effect size estimates and standard errors, MetaSex performs two sex-specific tests and a random effects model meta-analysis.
Applying this full model to GWAS can be computationally challenging because there are five variance components to fit (
and
). Currently available linear mixed model methods for association mapping are optimized for models with two variance components (Kang et al. 2008, 2010; Zhou and Stephens 2012). If there is a third component, a state-of-the-art method uses a simple grid search (Lippert et al. 2011). Thus, fitting five variance components may require a three-dimensional grid search, which can be prohibitively slow for GWAS.
To expedite the application of our model to GWAS, we propose an efficient decomposition of the model. Suppose that we restrict our scope to individuals of one sex. Then, the full model with five variance components collapses into a sex-specific model with two variance components (Figure 1B). Thus, the model can be efficiently solved using existing approaches (Kang et al. 2008, 2010; Zhou and Stephens 2012). In the decomposed model, we cannot distinguish the whole-sample polygenic component () from the sex-specific polygenic components (
or
) because they follow exactly the same distribution conditioned on one sex. However, this distinction is unimportant for association mapping, because we want to control for both.
Finally, given the sex-specific effect size estimates and the standard errors [
], we apply a series of statistical tests. We first apply SST, which is optimal for detecting sex-specific effects. Then, to effectively detect sex-interacting effects, we combine the two sex-specific estimates using the RE (Han and Eskin 2011) (Figure 1C), which explicitly models heterogeneity. As a result, our framework involves three tests (female SST, male SST, and RE), requiring multiple testing correction. A powerful multiple testing strategy can be to adjust the significance threshold for each test to maximize power while controlling for overall false positive rate (Eskin 2008). We identify and propose a set of thresholds for the three tests, what we call smart thresholding, that exactly controls the false positive rate to the GWAS threshold (
) while maximizing power.
Linear mixed model:
Our MetaSex framework is based on a linear mixed model designed to account for the sex difference in genetic architectures. The standard linear mixed model to account for the polygenic background effects is:(1)where y is a phenotype vector, μ is an intercept, 1 is a vector of ones, x is a genotype vector, β is the genetic effect,
is a variance component that accounts for the polygenic effects, and
is the random error term. Recent studies have developed numerical optimization strategies that allow an efficient application of this model to GWAS (Kang et al. 2008, 2010; Zhou and Stephens 2012).
We expand this model to account for the potential sex differences where we assume that each of the four terms of the standard model (intercept, genetic effect, polygenic effect, and error variance) can have differences between the sexes. The expanded model is:(2)where h is the sex status indicator,
and
As we described in the Overview section,
is the standard variance component that accounts for the polygenic effects,
is an additional variance component that accounts for the female-specific sex-interacting polygenic effects,
is a variance component that accounts for the male-specific sex-interacting polygenic effects, and
and
are sex-specific error terms that account for the difference in error variances between the sexes.
Because this comprehensive model involves five variance components, application of this model to GWAS can be computationally challenging. For this reason, we apply the following approximation and split the model into two sex-specific models:(3)where
is the phenotype vector of female individuals,
is the effect size in females,
is the genotype vector of female individuals,
is the polygenic effect within females, and
is the female-specific error term.
is the genotype similarity matrix between female individuals and
is an identity matrix defined for the female sample size. We similarly define terms for males. This approximated model has the following relationship to the previous full model:
These equalities hold because the approximated model can be considered as the same comprehensive model where we only look at a subset of samples (one sex). Intuitively, since we separate each sex into two models, the intercept is no more tied to be the same between the two sexes. This freedom accounts for the phenotypic mean difference between the sexes. This is the same for the genetic effect size (β) and the error variance. The polygenic effect term for each sex simultaneously accounts for both the whole-sample polygenic and the sex-interacting polygenic effects in the original model because, for each sex, the covariance matrices of the two terms become identical.
The benefit of this approximated model is that each model contains only two variance components. Currently available methods are well optimized for this two-variance-component model (Kang et al. 2008, 2010; Zhou and Stephens 2012). The difference in this approximated model compared with the original model is that, in the original model,
, and
are separately estimated, allowing for the distinction between the three. By contrast, in this approximated model, the estimates
and
do not allow distinction between the whole-sample polygenic component and the sex-interacting polygenic component. However, this distinction is not crucial in association mapping, where we want to control for both effects. Another difference is that the cross-sex elements in K are not used, which would give more accurate estimates of variance components. However, if the sample size in each sex is sufficiently large, the variance component estimates of the approximated model will almost be identical to those of the original model. We also note that the cryptic relatedness between the sexes are not accounted for in this approximated model.
SST:
In our framework, after obtaining the effect size estimate and its SE from each sex-specific model (for example, and
for females), we first apply the SST. The null hypothesis of SST is that the variant has no effect in each sex (
). We can obtain a P-value from female-specific test [SST(F)] and a P-value from male-specific test [SST(M)]. Since we perform two independent tests in SST, we correct for multiple testing. The reason that we apply SST first is not only because SST is optimal for detecting sex-specific effects, but also because in practice, investigators typically look at each sex separately in their data. By explicitly including this test in our framework, we can account for multiple testing induced by this test.
Whole-sample test using meta-analysis:
The next step of our framework is to perform a whole-sample test by combining information from both sexes. Our goal is to find a locus that has either common effect (effect that exists for both sexes with the same effect size) or interaction effect (effect that exists for both sexes with differing effect sizes). In the comprehensive model, our null hypothesis is In our approximated model, this null hypothesis translates to an equivalent null hypothesis,
What would be an optimal approach for simultaneously testing
and
will depend on the alternative models. If
and
are expected to be completely different (e.g., opposite directions of effects), simply adding χ2 statistics as is done in the genome-wide association meta-analysis method (GWAMA) (Magi et al. 2010) would be powerful. More common situations would be that the effects are in the same direction but in different magnitudes. Nevertheless, if the magnitudes of effects are extremely different such that one effect is relatively very close to zero, then the variant is likely to be already found by SST. Thus, we can specifically target effect size pairs whose directions are the same and whose magnitudes can be different, but none is very close to zero. To this end, we chose to use the RE which assumes that the male and female effect sizes are random variables drawn from the same underlying distribution.
The traditional RE model assumes that the effect size of each study, follows a distribution with the grand mean
and the variance
(DerSimonian and Laird 1986; Han and Eskin 2011):
The recently proposed RE model by Han and Eskin (2011) tests the null hypothesis
vs. the alternative hypothesis
The difference from the traditional RE model is that the Han–Eskin model assumes no heterogeneity under the null hypothesis (Han and Eskin 2011). This assumption is valid if the causes of heterogeneity do not exist under the null hypothesis, which is likely to be the case for GWAS. Han and Eskin built a likelihood ratio test of which likelihood functions of the null and alternative hypotheses are (Han and Eskin 2011):
To apply the RE model by Han and Eskin to our framework, we assumed a meta-analysis combining two pairs of observations
(
), whose variances are
Then, applying RE is equivalent to estimating the grand mean of the genetic effects between sexes
and the between-sex heterogeneity
from the likelihood functions above and testing the null hypothesis
vs. the alternative hypothesis
Note that this null hypothesis exactly corresponds to
We can rewrite the likelihood functions as follows:
The maximum likelihood estimates
and
can be found by an iterative procedure suggested by Hardy and Thompson (1996). Then the likelihood ratio test statistic can be built:
(4)which asymptotically follows a half and half mixture of
and
under the null. The P-value after a small sample adjustment can be efficiently calculated using a precomputed table (Han and Eskin 2011).
Smart thresholding:
In our MetaSex framework, we perform three tests: SST, which consists of SST(F) and SST(M), and the whole sample test using RE. To account for multiple testing, we can use the Bonferroni correction, but that can be overly conservative because of the dependency between the test statistics of SST and RE. Instead, we can perform null simulations to empirically determine the significance threshold. Moreover, we can use a strategy similar to one published previously (Eskin 2008), which uses different levels of significance thresholds for multiple tests to achieve higher power while controlling the overall false positive rate (family-wise error rate) to a fixed level.
To find an optimal threshold pair for RE and SST while controlling the false positive rate at we generated 10 billion null statistic pairs for the male studies and the female studies. Any pair of thresholds for RE and SST that rejected 500 null statistics would control the false positive rate at
Then, we adjusted the thresholds for RE and SST while keeping the total number of rejections to 500. For example, a threshold pair can have one false positive for RE and 499 false positives for SST. Next, one can have two false positives for RE and 498 false positives for SST. There were 500 such threshold pairs that control the false positive rate of
Among all 500 pairs of thresholds that gave the same false positive rate, we chose the threshold pair that gave us the maximum power. To calculate power, we needed an assumption for the alternative hypothesis. We assumed a model in Figure 4, which uniformly sampled the female effect size and male effect size from a range between 0 and 1. Although this alternative model was just one possible model, we expect that it will cover a range of possible situations. Under this uniform prior assumption, we calculated the power of each pair. We found that using unequal thresholds,
for RE and
for SST(F) and SST(M), gave us the best power while still controlling the false positive rate. Note that although our pair of thresholds was optimized for a specific alternative model, even if the true alternative model would be different from the assumed model, our false positive rate can be still controlled; only the power will be affected. The users using our method can just use these precomputed thresholds.
Existing approaches
CV:
The standard approach for dealing with sex is to use sex as a CV. We refer to this model as CV in short. The CV model is:which is equivalent to the traditional model in Equation (1), with the only difference being the inclusion of the CV denoting sex (
). CV accounts for the phenotypic mean difference between the sexes. However, CV does not account for the potential sex difference in the effect sizes (β), polygenic background effects (u), and the error variances [
].
GWAMA:
GWAMA is another meta-analytic approach proposed by Magi et al. (2010). In GWAMA, as in MetaSex, each sex is analyzed separately. Then, the statistics of males and females are calculated by squaring the corresponding z-scores, that is:
The GWAMA statistic can be obtained by summing the male
and female
:
The P-value can be obtained from a
distribution with two degrees of freedom.
Because GWAMA is a meta-analytic approach that analyzes each sex separately and combines summary statistics from the two sexes, it shares some of the advantages with our MetaSex approach. That is, GWAMA framework can account for between-sex differences in intercepts and error variances.
Note that N degrees of freedom χ2 test for N strata is a general method that was used in many other contexts such as for performing a joint test for genetic main effects and gene × environment interaction effects (Aschard et al. 2010). Other studies often call this test an “N d.f. test” (Aschard et al. 2010; Winkler et al. 2015).
Power calculation
To evaluate the power of methods, we performed simulations as follows. We assumed a specific effect size. Then based on an assumed SE, we sampled an observed estimate of effect size. We performed this sampling for males and females separately by M times. Given M male estimates and M female estimates, we applied each of the tested methods. The statistical power was computed as the proportion of P-values that were more significant than a significance threshold. We found the method-specific significance threshold by performing null simulations under the null hypothesis of no effects; Empirical null simulation was necessary because some methods involved multiple testing. For example, SST consists of two tests, SST(F) and SST(M), and MetaSex (RE + SST) consists of three tests, SST(F), SST(M), and RE. As with MetaSex, each of GWAMA + SST and CV + SST consists of three tests. We used M of at least 10,000 in all of our simulations.
Levene’s test
Levene’s test determines if there is a significant difference among the variances of multiple groups (Brown and Forsythe 1974). The statistic is:(5)where k is the number of different groups to which the samples belong (
for our between-sex test), N is the total number of samples, and
is the number of samples in the ith group. Let
be the value of the measured variable for the jth sample from the ith group. We define
(
is a mean of ith group),
and
The resulting statistic W follows an F distribution with
and
degrees of freedom under the null hypothesis.
NFBC data
In our current study, we used the previously reported NFBC data (Sabatti et al. 2009), which contained 5326 individuals (2546 males and 2780 females). To investigate the sex difference in genetic architectures of human traits, we examined 10 phenotypes: triglycerides, high-density lipoprotein (HDL), low-density lipoprotein (LDL), C-reactive protein, glucose, insulin, BMI, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, and height. Detailed trait measurements and sample genotype collection have previously been described (Sabatti et al. 2009).
Data availability
The RE used in the MetaSex framework is publicly available at http://genetics.cs.ucla.edu/meta/. The authors affirm that all data necessary for confirming the conclusions of the article are present within the article, figures, and tables. Supplemental material available at Figshare: https://doi.org/10.25386/genetics.6071567.
Results
Power comparison
Simulation setting:
We performed simulations to evaluate the power of our MetaSex approach. Below, we also refer to our MetaSex method as RE + SST because the framework involves simultaneous testing of RE and SST. We compared our method to two other approaches: (1) CV, the traditional approach using sex as a CV, and (2) GWAMA (Magi et al. 2010), another meta-analysis approach designed to discover sex-interacting effects. CV and GWAMA are similar to RE in that they use the whole sample. We assumed a practical situation in which investigators examine each sex separately, using SST regardless of which method is used for the whole sample. Thus, we compared the power of our MetaSex (RE + SST) approach with that of CV + SST and GWAMA + SST, where A + B denotes a combination method that calls a result significant if either A or B method gives a significant result after correcting for multiple testing. We also compared the power of the bare SST to get a sense of how much power is increased by the methods using the whole sample.
To make a fair comparison between these methods, we corrected for multiple testing within each method in an equitable way. In MetaSex (RE + SST), CV + SST, and GWAMA + SST, we performed three tests, whereas in SST, we performed two tests. Therefore, for each of these methods, we generated 10 billion () null male/female statistic pairs and chose the 500th smallest P-value, which was the method-specific significance threshold to control the false positive rate (family-wise error rate) to
The resulting significance thresholds were
for CV + SST,
for GWAMA + SST, and
for SST. For MetaSex (RE + SST), we used our smart thresholding strategy that applied
for RE and
for each of the two SST (see Materials and Methods). These empirically calculated thresholds ensured that the false positive rates of all compared methods were well controlled. See Materials and Methods for the further details of our power simulations.
Simulating the sex difference in effect sizes:
In the first power simulation, we simulated the effect size difference between the sexes, a phenomenon called “effect size heterogeneity.” We assumed a SNP of minor allele frequency 0.3 and generated genotypes of 1000 males and 1000 females. Then we simulated continuous phenotypes of these individuals while assuming the same error variance for the two sexes. For each male individual, we generated a phenotype assuming a genetic effect size of 0.192 and variance of 1.0. For each female individual, we generated a phenotype assuming 10% of the male effect size (0.019) and variance of 1.0. We repeated this simulation 10,000 times and computed the power of a method as the proportion of simulations in which the test P-value was more significant than the given significance threshold. We then gradually increased the female effect size from 10 to 100% of the male effect size, to simulate differing levels of heterogeneity.
Figure 2 shows the power of the four approaches (RE + SST, CV + SST, GWAMA + SST, and SST) with respect to the effect size ratio between the two sexes. As expected, when the effect size ratio was very small (when the effect was almost sex-specific), SST showed the highest statistical power. As the effect size of the female study increased, the MetaSex (RE + SST) approach showed the highest statistical power, demonstrating that our approach can effectively detect sex-interacting effects. Even at the ratio of 1.0 (when the effect size was identical for both sexes), although CV was expected to be the most powerful, MetaSex (RE + SST) slightly outperformed CV + SST. This was because of our smart thresholding strategy that allowed a more liberal significance threshold for RE with the expense of a more stringent threshold for SST.
Power comparison for the MetaSex (RE + SST), CV + SST, GWAMA + SST, and SST approaches where we varied the effect size ratios of females and males. All methods were corrected for multiple testing.
Simulating the sex difference in error variances:
In the second power simulation, we simulated the error variance difference between the sexes while assuming a constant effect size (no heterogeneity). As in the first simulation, for each male and female individual, we generated a phenotype assuming a genetic effect size of 0.2 and variance of 1.0. Then, we gradually increased the error variance of the females from 1.0 to 4.0 (SD from 1.0 to 2.0). Figure 3 shows the power of the four approaches (RE + SST, CV + SST, GWAMA + SST, and SST) with respect to the SD ratio between the two sexes. Our proposed approach (RE + SST) outperformed other methods in all simulated situations. When we examined the second and the third best methods, we observed that GWAMA + SST outperformed CV + SST when the SD ratio was large (). This was because GWAMA, being a meta-analytic approach that estimates the variance of error terms in each sex separately, was robust to the variance difference between the sexes. Although both our MetaSex (RE + SST) and GWAMA + SST were meta-analytic methods, our method consistently outperformed GWAMA + SST. In addition, we simulated a power comparison where we simultaneously varied the effect size ratios and the variance ratios between females and males. In this simulation, RE + SST was still the most powerful (Supplemental Material, Figure S1).
Power comparison for the MetaSex (RE + SST), CV + SST, GWAMA + SST, and SST approaches where we varied the error variance ratio in females and males. All methods were corrected for multiple testing.
Power characteristics of the methods:
We examined why using RE to complement SST was more powerful than using GWAMA or CV to complement SST. We evaluated the power of the individual methods (RE, CV, GWAMA, and SST) over a wide range of female/male genetic effect size pairs, varying each from small value (0) to large value (1.0). Note that although we examined a specific effect size range (0, 1.0), the general tendencies in relative power are expected to be similar in different settings; for example, if the effect sizes were larger and the variances were larger, the power results would be similar. Here, we assumed an error variance ratio of 1.2 (females/males) between the two sexes, because this was the average ratio of the phenotypic variances in the 10 phenotypes of the NFBC data.
Figure 4 shows the power of the four individual methods (RE, CV, SST, and GWAMA) in a two-dimensional space where x-axis is the male genetic effect size and y-axis is the female genetic effect size of a SNP. We plotted the power lines of the four methods, so that each line denotes pairs of the male and female effect sizes where the method achieved an exact power of 50%. Because the power increased as the effect size increased, the closer the
line was to the bottom leftmost point on the graph, the more powerful the method was. As expected, when one of the effect sizes was close to zero (sex-specific effect: top left corner or bottom right corner), SST was the most powerful. When the effect sizes were at most moderately different between male and female studies (middle area), RE outperformed other approaches. We measured the size of the area whose power was >
which was the area outside of each curve, toward the top right corner. The sizes of the areas were
for SST,
for CV,
for GWAMA, and
for RE. Thus, RE and GWAMA achieved the largest similar areas.
Power characteristics of RE, CV, GWAMA, and SST in a space where we varied the effect sizes of males and females. Each line denotes the effect size pairs for which a method achieved 50% power. We assumed the error variance ratio of 1.2 between males and females. The diagonal line shows the points where the effect sizes of the two sexes were equal.
Figure 4 demonstrates why using RE to complement SST allowed us better power than using GWAMA or CV to complement SST in previous simulations (Figure 2 and Figure 3). The GWAMA power line was more steeply curved than the RE power line, which meant that GWAMA tended to detect effects that were extremely different between the sexes. Therefore, what GWAMA found could have substantial overlap to what SST found. The power of the combined methods (RE + SST, CV + SST, and GWAMA + SST) can be interpreted as the sum of areas where each method achieved the power of or more. Figure 4 shows that RE and SST complemented each other resulting in the largest combined area in this plot. To quantify this difference, we measured the area >
not covered by the
power of the SST approach. The areas were
for GWAMA and
for RE. Thus, in the common situations that investigators apply SST first, RE can give us the biggest additional power.
We then tested if the three-method compositions, such as RE + CV + SST, RE + GWAMA + SST, and CV + GWAMA + SST, can have better power. We assumed the female effect size of 0.125 and male effect size of 0.25 (effect size ratio of 0.5). We applied the Bonferroni correction to the three-method compositions. Figure S2 shows the power comparison of the seven methods (RE + SST, CV + SST, GWAMA + SST, SST, RE + CV + SST, RE + GWAMA + SST, and CV + GWAMA + SST), where MetaSex (RE + SST) slightly outperformed the others.
Analysis of the NFBC data
We analyzed the NFBC data (Sabatti et al. 2009), which consisted of 5326 individuals (2546 males and 2780 females). This dataset provided 10 phenotypic measurements of the individuals (see Materials and Methods).
Sex difference in phenotypic variances:
We first investigated whether the phenotypic variances showed differences between the sexes. We applied Levene’s test, which tests for the equality of the variances between two groups (see Materials and Methods). Table 1 shows that five phenotypes (triglycerides, HDL, LDL, BMI, and diastolic blood pressure) showed significant differences in the phenotypic variance between females and males ( Bonferroni correction on 10 tests). The most significant difference was observed in triglycerides (
).
Variance component analysis:
To investigate why the phenotypic variances of some traits differed between the sexes, we performed a variance component analysis. We used the five-variance-component linear mixed model described in Figure 1A, where we excluded the SNP terms. Decomposing the variance components could reveal if the phenotypic variance difference came from the differences in polygenic background effects, or the differences in error variances. We used the Genome-wide Complex Trait Analysis (GCTA) method to perform this analysis (Yang et al. 2010). First, we generated the genetic relationship matrix, using the GCTA framework. Second, we created two modified genetic relationship matrices,
and
by masking the values of K except for the sex-specific values (see Materials and Methods). Unfortunately, GCTA did not allow us to separate the error term into two sex-specific terms as in the model in Figure 1A, because the default error term (with variance
) for the whole sample was automatically included in the model. Thus, we added the sex-specific error term (with variance
) to one sex. We tried both males and females for this additional term, and chose the configuration with
Table 2 and Table S1 show the variance component estimates. The polygenic background effect () was significantly nonzero in five traits (
for BMI, HDL, height, LDL, and systolic blood pressure). The sex-interacting polygenic background effects (
and
) were nonzero in some traits, but the SEs were large and none of them showed significance (
). The variance of the sex-specific error term (
) was significantly nonzero in traits triglycerides (
) and BMI (
). Thus, in some phenotypes, the phenotypic variance difference between the sexes was not completely explained by the genetic components alone, which suggested the need for explicitly modeling the sex difference in error variances as in our MetaSex method.
Full model vs. approximated model:
Although it was feasible to estimate the five variance components one time using tools such as GCTA, applying the full linear mixed model to millions of markers can be prohibitively slow because the variance component estimation needs to be repeated for each SNP. Therefore, we proposed an approximated model that decomposes the problem into two sex-specific linear mixed models (Figure 1B). Here, using the NFBC dataset, we examined if the variance components estimated by the approximated model were similar to those estimated by the full model.
To achieve this goal, we performed GCTA analyses in each sex-specific two-variance-component model described in Figure 1B. Table 3 shows the estimated variance components for the two sexes. We can categorize the variance components into two groups: the genetic variance and the random error variance. The genetic variances in the full and sex-specific models are from Table 2 and
from Table 3, respectively. The random error variances in the full and sex-specific models are
from Table 2 (or
depending on which sex
was added) and
from Table 3, respectively. We examined if the genetic and random error variances were the same between the full and sex-specific models. Figure 5 shows that the estimated variance components were highly concordant between the full and sex-specific models. Most of the points closely followed the
line (dashed line). The Pearson correlations were high (
) in all comparisons, except for the male genetic variance. The low correlation in the male genetic variance was driven by one outlier (diastolic blood pressure). This outlier appears to have a large SE of the estimates. Specifically, the SE of the genetic variance
in the sex-specific model was 12.47, which was five orders of magnitude greater than the estimate itself (Table 2). If we excluded this outlier, the correlation was high (
). Overall, the estimates of the full and sex-specific models were highly concordant, which supported the use of the approximated model in our framework.
Comparison of the variance components between the full and the sex-specific models for the 10 phenotypes of NFBC data. The points represent phenotypes. For each point, the vertical and horizontal lines represent the 95% confidence intervals in the full and sex-specific models, respectively. The dotted line is where the variance estimates of the two models are equal. We evaluated in the
scale and labeled an outlier (diastolic blood pressure, red color) observed in the male genetic variance plot (bottom left).
Association mapping:
We mapped associations for the 10 phenotypes in the NFBC dataset, using different methods. We used our efficient approximated model; that is, in each sex, we applied a sex-specific linear mixed model (Efficient Mixed-Model Association eXpedited (EMMAX); Kang et al. 2010) to account for the polygenic effects and the sex-interacting polygenic effect simultaneously (Figure 1B). Then, we applied RE and SST to the resulting female and male effect size estimates. For comparison, we also applied CV and GWAMA. In both CV and GWAMA, we similarly accounted for the polygenic background effects using variance components. Finally, for a fair comparison, we calculated the genomic control factor λ separately for each method (Table S2) and corrected the resulting P-values of each method, using this factor.
The challenge in this real dataset analysis was the lack of an objective measure to compare performances of the methods because we do not know which loci are true positives. What we could do was to examine loci that were genome-wide significant and compare the P-values of different methods. Under the assumption that the loci exceeding the significance threshold have a high chance of being true positives, a putatively better method can be a method that gave smaller P-values at those loci.
In this analysis, we discovered 16 loci that were associated with any of the 10 phenotypes at the threshold level by at least one method. At these 16 loci, we calculated the P-values using RE, CV, GWAMA, and SST (Table S3). To compare the P-values of the methods, we chose CV as a reference method. We plotted the
difference between each method and the CV approach in Figure 6A [(
of RE/GWAMA/SST) − (
of CV)]. Thus, for each SNP, the positively larger the difference, the better the method was compared with CV. As shown in Figure 6A, RE showed the best overall performance. RE gave smaller P-values than GWAMA at 14 out of 16 loci and better P-values than CV at 11 out of 16 loci. Even at loci where GWAMA or CV showed smaller P-values than RE, the difference from RE was small. Specifically, RE P-values were never larger by one order of magnitude than any of these methods at all 16 loci.
Association results of RE, CV, GWAMA, and SST in NFBC data. We show 16 SNPs that were associated to one of the 10 phenotypes. (A) Relative improvement of the other methods compared with the CV method [(
of RE/GWAMA/SST) − (
of CV)]. The reference SNP identity (RSID) of the SNPs as well as their CV P-values are shown at the bottom. (B) The ratio of the phenotypic variance between males and females after regressing out the genetic effect of each SNP. (C) The ratio of the genetic effect size of each SNP between males and females. CRP, C-reactive protein.
We further investigated on what characteristics of the loci caused these P-value differences between the methods. First, Figure 6B shows the phenotype variance ratio (PVR) between males and females after regressing out the genetic effect of the SNP tested. Second, Figure 6C shows the effect size ratio between males and females for each SNP. Because the power of the methods depend on both the error variance ratio (which will affect PVR) and the effect size ratio between males and females as we have shown in simulations, we can interpret the P-values of the methods (Figure 6A) in terms of the PVR (Figure 6B) and the effect size ratio (Figure 6C).
If we look at the SNP rs7298683 (indicated by †, Figure 6), the effect size ratio between males and females was −0.0227 (Table S3), which meant that the effect direction was opposite for the two sexes and that the absolute magnitude of the male effect size was 36 times larger than that of the female effect size. However, there was almost no difference in PVR between male and female studies (PVR of 0.965). In this case, the SST approach gave the smallest P-value, because SST was the best method to detect an extreme effect size magnitude difference as we have shown in simulations.
If we look at the SNP rs2167079 (indicated by *, Figure 6), the PVR (female/male) was 1.261 and the effect size ratio (female/male) was 0.43 (Table S3). The variance in females was larger, so the female effect size estimate was more uncertain than the male estimate. Thus, when combining information from the two sexes, an optimal method should give more weight to male estimate. Moreover, effect size was greater in males. Thus, an optimal method should give more weight to the male estimate even further. Because CV ignores both the variance difference and the effect size difference, RE achieved a smaller P-value at this locus than CV. A similar interpretation of the result can be applied to the SNPs rs7120118 and rs693 (indicated by ●, Figure 6).
Now consider the SNP rs11668477 (indicated by ‡, Figure 6). The PVR (female/male) was 0.82 and the effect size ratio (female/male) was 0.5 (Table S3). In this case, when combining information from the two sexes, based on the variance, we should weight the female estimate, but based on the effect size, we should weight the male estimate. Thus, the effect of differing variances and the effect of differing effect sizes canceled out, giving CV the smallest P-value of all approaches because CV can be considered as equally weighting the two sexes. A similar interpretation of the result can be applied to the SNPs rs2794520, rs560887, and rs10096633 (indicated by ⋄, Figure 6). However, as described, even in such situations, RE was not much worse than CV.
In summary, RE showed the best stable performance of all methods, except when the effect only existed in one sex where SST performed the best. This analysis demonstrates that our MetaSex framework, where RE and SST complement each other, can cover many possible situations with high power.
Discussion
Here, we present MetaSex, a novel framework that accounts for the potential sex difference in genetic architectures for powerful association mapping. We built our method on a comprehensive model that included multiple variance components and expedited the optimization by using an approximated sex-specific models. We utilized the meta-analysis framework to achieve high power in a wide range of situations. Simulations and real data analyses supported the superior performance of our approach compared with previous approaches.
The high power of our approach was attributable to two factors: the effect size difference between the sexes and the error variance difference between the sexes. Previous studies have observed effect size differences at a number of loci (Randall et al. 2013; Winkler et al. 2015). However, few studies have reported phenotypic variance differences between the sexes, which can reflect the error variance difference. In our study, we showed that the phenotypic variance difference can be a real phenomenon in the existing dataset. The nongenetic cause of the phenotypic variance difference can be sex acting as an environment (e.g., hormone) or sex interacting with external environments (e.g., lifestyle). We demonstrated that accounting for the nongenetic causes by modeling differing error variances can increase power.
Our framework can be generalized to analyze data containing any strata other than the sex. To apply our framework to N strata, we can apply the linear mixed model to each stratum and obtain N effect size estimates and their SE. First, we can perform stratum-specific tests with the estimated effect sizes. Next, we use those estimates as an input to RE and perform a meta-analysis for multiple strata. Finally, we can correct for multiple testing of the RE and N stratum-specific tests by the smart thresholding strategy, where the thresholds can be calculated for a specific situation.
In our simulations, we assumed that the effects of the two sexes are in the same direction. Our assumption was that for disease phenotypes, it would be rare that the same variant increases risk for one sex and decreases for the other. The results of the NFBC dataset supported this assumption, given that none of the 16 associated variants showed significant evidence of opposite effects (Table S3). In a recent study, some variants were found to be associated to waist/hip ratio in males and females in an opposite way (Winkler et al. 2015). We tried an extended simulation setup to test variants with opposite effects (Figure S3). Among the four approaches (RE + SST, CV + SST, GWAMA + SST, and SST), GWAMA + SST achieved the highest power if the directions were opposite. This indicates that RE and GWAMA can play a different role in the analysis; RE is powerful for detecting unidirectional effects while GWAMA is powerful for detecting opposite effects.
We did not systematically compare the runtime of the methods (MetaSex, CV + SST, and GWAMA + SST) because the runtime greatly depends on the specific implementation of the linear mixed model that is applied to each sex. Because the meta-analytic part (RE or GWAMA) only combines two estimates (female and male effect sizes), the runtime for meta-analysis is relatively negligible. Zhou and Stephens compared the runtimes of the standard linear mixed models for two variance components (Zhou and Stephens 2012), which showed that the recent implementations of the linear mixed model can be applied to the typical GWAS dataset within a single day. Therefore, if we use these implementations for each sex, the time complexity of the whole procedure of our framework will be similar.
Although we tried to account for many possible phenomena that can occur due to the sex difference in genetic association mapping, our model might still have limitations. In our method, we explicitly modeled the sex difference in the effect sizes of the associated locus, magnitudes of the polygenic effects, and error variances. However, we did not model the sex difference in the phenotype distribution (i.e., shape), genetic interaction with CVs, or the liability distribution of binary traits. Moreover, we only assumed a specific parameter space or dataset both in power evaluations and in variance estimate comparison of the full and approximated models. In future analyses, extended datasets or simulations may help us evaluate the full characteristics of our method in the wider spectrum of situations. We expect that a large-scale study will be necessary to fully decipher sex-interacting genetic architectures of human traits.
Acknowledgments
C.H.L. and B.H. are supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant (grant number 2016R1C1B2013126) and the Bio & Medical Technology Development Program of the NRF (grant number 2017M3A9B6061852) funded by the Korean government, Ministry of Science and ICT. E.Y.K., N.A.F., J.W.J.J., E.K., N.Z., and E.E. are supported by National Science Foundation grants 0513612, 0731455, 0729049, 0916676, 1065276, 1302448, 1320589, and 1331176, and National Institutes of Health grants K25-HL080079, U01-DA024417, P01-HL30568, P01-HL28481, R01-GM083198, R01-ES021801, R01-MH101782, and R01-ES022282. The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
↵Supplemental material available at Figshare: https://doi.org/10.25386/genetics.6071567.
Communicating editor: C. Sabatti
- Received November 10, 2017.
- Accepted April 12, 2018.
- Copyright © 2018 by the Genetics Society of America