Skip to main content
  • Facebook
  • Twitter
  • YouTube
  • LinkedIn
  • Google Plus
  • Other GSA Resources
    • Genetics Society of America
    • G3: Genes | Genomes | Genetics
    • Genes to Genomes: The GSA Blog
    • GSA Conferences
    • GeneticsCareers.org
  • Log in
Genetics

Main menu

  • HOME
  • ISSUES
    • Current Issue
    • Early Online
    • Archive
  • ABOUT
    • About the journal
    • Why publish with us?
    • Editorial board
    • Contact us
  • SERIES
    • Centennial
    • Genetics of Immunity
    • Genetics of Sex
    • Genomic Selection
    • Multiparental Populations
    • FlyBook
    • WormBook
    • YeastBook
  • ARTICLE TYPES
    • About Article Types
    • Commentaries
    • Editorials
    • GSA Honors and Awards
    • Methods, Technology & Resources
    • Perspectives
    • Primers
    • Reviews
    • Toolbox Reviews
  • PUBLISH & REVIEW
    • Scope & publication policies
    • Submission & review process
    • Article types
    • Prepare your manuscript
    • Submit your manuscript
    • After acceptance
    • Guidelines for reviewers
  • SUBSCRIBE
    • Why subscribe?
    • For institutions
    • For individuals
    • Email alerts
    • RSS feeds
  • Other GSA Resources
    • Genetics Society of America
    • G3: Genes | Genomes | Genetics
    • Genes to Genomes: The GSA Blog
    • GSA Conferences
    • GeneticsCareers.org

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
Genetics

Advanced Search

  • HOME
  • ISSUES
    • Current Issue
    • Early Online
    • Archive
  • ABOUT
    • About the journal
    • Why publish with us?
    • Editorial board
    • Contact us
  • SERIES
    • Centennial
    • Genetics of Immunity
    • Genetics of Sex
    • Genomic Selection
    • Multiparental Populations
    • FlyBook
    • WormBook
    • YeastBook
  • ARTICLE TYPES
    • About Article Types
    • Commentaries
    • Editorials
    • GSA Honors and Awards
    • Methods, Technology & Resources
    • Perspectives
    • Primers
    • Reviews
    • Toolbox Reviews
  • PUBLISH & REVIEW
    • Scope & publication policies
    • Submission & review process
    • Article types
    • Prepare your manuscript
    • Submit your manuscript
    • After acceptance
    • Guidelines for reviewers
  • SUBSCRIBE
    • Why subscribe?
    • For institutions
    • For individuals
    • Email alerts
    • RSS feeds
Previous ArticleNext Article

The Genetics of Canine Skull Shape Variation

Jeffrey J. Schoenebeck and Elaine A. Ostrander
Genetics February 1, 2013 vol. 193 no. 2 317-325; https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.112.145284
Jeffrey J. Schoenebeck
Cancer Genetics Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Elaine A. Ostrander
Cancer Genetics Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: eostrand@mail.nih.gov
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
Loading

Abstract

A dog’s craniofacial diversity is the result of continual human intervention in natural selection, a process that began tens of thousands of years ago. To date, we know little of the genetic underpinnings and developmental mechanisms that make dog skulls so morphologically plastic. In this Perspectives, we discuss the origins of dog skull shapes in terms of history and biology and highlight recent advances in understanding the genetics of canine skull shapes. Of particular interest are those molecular genetic changes that are associated with the development of distinct breeds.

  • dog
  • craniofacial
  • genome-wide association studies (GWAS)
  • genetics
  • morphology

SOMETIME during the Paleolithic, a remarkable transformation occurred. Small numbers of gray wolves adopted a new pack master—humans. Through the process of domestication, the modern dog emerged. Today most dogs share little resemblance to their lupine ancestors. As a result of artificial selection, dogs radiated to fill niches in our lives, becoming our herders, guardians, hunters, rescuers, and companions (Wilcox and Walkowicz 1995). The range of sizes among dogs extends beyond that of wolves, giving dogs the distinction of being the most morphologically diverse terrestrial mammalian species known (Stockard 1941). Equally dramatic to humans’ effect on scale, is the effect on the dog’s facial features, particularly the skull (Figure 1).

Figure 1 
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1 

A montage of canine craniofacial shape demonstrates the incredible morphologic diversity of Canis familiaris. Dorsal, lateral, and ventral perspectives of various breeds of dogs. Lateral views are articulated so that the skull base (red line, wolf) is approximately parallel between breeds. Prominent differences across breeds include palate shape (p, indicated by white dots), neurocranium shape (nc, enclosed by blue dots), cranial base length (cb, red line). Also note the angle of the palate relative to the cranial base.

Given its prominence as a hallmark for domestication and its indication of breed identity, we devote the remainder of this review to a discussion of our current understanding of canine skull diversity and its mechanistic underpinnings as they relate to domestication, genetics, and disease. Although every attempt is made to keep our discussion skull-centric, many of the topics broached are pertinent to other traits that distinguish different dog breeds, as are the genomic techniques currently employed to map and validate causal genetic variation underlying skull morphology. In such situations we have, of necessity, made our discussion broader to keep it inclusive.

Variation in Skull Shape and Dog Domestication

Molecular clock estimates from mitochondrial DNA suggest domestication started as early as 135,000 years ago (Vilà et al. 1997). More conservative estimates are based on archaeological records, which indicate that dog domestication began somewhere between 15,000 and 36,000 years ago (see summary by Larson et al. 2012). Current archaeological estimates depend on carbon dating of bones, whose morphologies appear distinct from that of contemporary wolves. In many cases, these distinctions are pronounced in both the skull and its dentition. This suggests that, among incipient dogs, the skull was at the leading edge of several anatomical changes that would transform wolves. For example, prehistoric dog skulls excavated in Russia were from massive animals that had shortened snouts and widened palates (Sablin and Khlopachev 2002). Elsewhere, ancient dogs were smaller than wolves (Napierala and Uerpmann 2010).

Like other large domesticates, modern dogs exhibit an increased brain-to-body-size ratio (termed encephalization). Yet the overall size of dogs’ brains relative to that of wolves has decreased by nearly 30% (Coppinger and Schneider 1995; Zeder 2012). This decrease is particularly acute in the limbic system, which is integral to fight or flight responses (Coppinger and Schneider 1995; Zeder 2012). Domestication may have, therefore, reduced areas of the wolf brain that enabled tolerance to human contact. It is likely that skull shape changed in response to changes in brain morphology. As proof of principle, during the 40 years it took to domesticate silver foxes, in a well-documented experiment performed in Novosibirsk, Russia, changes in cranial dimensions, among other morphologic features, were also noted as a correlate to tameness (Trut 1999; Zeder 2012).

Some have likened dogs to wolf pedomorphs, proposing that dogs are juvenilized wolves that are developmentally restrained in behavior and physical maturation (Gould 1977; Wayne 1986; Coppinger and Coppinger 2001; Drake and Klingenberg 2010; Drake 2011). Even in adulthood, a vast number of modern breeds display some sort of growth arrest, likened to wolf neoteny. With a few exceptions, modern dog breeds are smaller and have snouts and crania that are proportionally intermediate to wolf neonates and adults.

However, the idea that pedomorphism is a major driver of canine craniofacial variation is not without controversy. Certain skull characteristics, such as the angle between the palate and the neurocranium, appear static during wolf development, whereas in dogs the angle differs substantially from that of wolves’ throughout development, leading to the conclusion that dog skull shape is neomorphic (Drake 2011). This debate can be settled only by developing a genetic understanding of the variation underlying canine skull diversity. Identification of causative genetic mechanisms will also enable inference of the evolution of dog skull shape and provide insight regarding the demographics of wolves or ancestral dogs from which this variation emerged.

Emergence of Dog Breeds

By the 19th century, a new fad had swept across Europe that the British not only embraced, but also actively promoted. Dog fanciers began breeding and trading dogs that were “specialized” for both physical and behavioral traits. With the advent of the Kennel Club in 1873, lineages became standardized by appellation, bloodline, appearance, and behavior. Thus, to be recognized as a “bulldog,” it was insufficient for a dog to be squat in stature and display a shortened snout. Rather, a purebred bulldog had to conform to the club’s breed-specific standards, and, most importantly, the dog’s parents had to be club-registered bulldogs, ensuring bloodline purity.

Dog fanciers quickly recognized that structured breeding could be harnessed to transmit desirable traits. But how such selection could work on a grand scale was not immediately appreciated and attempts to parse the genetic mechanisms that dictate how canine traits such as skull shape are passed from parents to offspring were not formally studied until decades later. Most prominent were the studies conducted by Charles Stockard, whose detailed dog pedigrees include designed crosses and backcrosses of morphologically disparate breeds (Stockard 1941). On the basis of live observation and analysis of skeletal preparations of parents and progeny, Stockard concluded that breed-defining skull shape features, such as the bulldog’s shortened rostrum, did not follow patterns of Mendelian inheritance. Nearly 70 years would pass until genome-wide association studies (GWAS) would confirm Stockard’s predictions (Boyko et al. 2010; Schoenebeck et al. 2012).

Three factors were key to the acceptance of the dog as a system for studying genetics. First, geneticists recognized that different dog breeds were characterized by an enormous variety of genetically fixed morphologic traits whose genetic underpinnings were likely to enlighten our understanding of mammalian developmental biology. Second, it also became clear that the population structure of domestic breeds would allow geneticists to overcome many of the difficulties faced when doing either linkage or association studies in human populations. With >400 documented breeds worldwide, complex traits including morphologic traits such as body size, bone length and width, and skull shape, as well as disease susceptibility or even behavior could be disentangled by studying dog breeds, and the results would likely be applicable to other mammalian systems, including human (Karlsson and Lindblad-Toh 2008; Shearin and Ostrander 2010; Ostrander 2012). Finally, it became clear that the genomic methods under development for navigating the mouse and human genomes were readily transferable to studying the dog genome. The rapid development of both genetic (Mellersh et al. 2000) and physical (Guyon et al. 2003) maps of the dog, followed by a 7.5× sequence and draft assembly (Lindblad-Toh et al. 2005), put the dog on par with traditional model organisms for performing genetic studies.

Modern Domestic Dog Breed Variation

Today, >400 breeds of dogs exist worldwide. Together, the morphological variation among these breeds is so diverse and readily discernible that, for many, skull shape is breed-defining (Figure 1). Some canine skull conformations are named after their resemblance to human craniosynostoses, such as brachycephaly and dolichocephaly (Figure 2). Brachycephalic breeds, such as the bulldog, pug, and Boston terrier, are easily recognized by their short “pushed-in” faces, underbite, and widely placed, shallow orbits. Brachycephaly, which means “short head,” is a term borrowed from human medicine. In dogs, a number of craniofacial anomalies can contribute to brachycephaly, including a reduction in the length of bones that form the rostrum, chondrodysplasia of the cranial base, and changes in the palate position relative to the cranial base (Figure 2; Huber 1974; Nussbaumer 1978).

Figure 2 
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2 

Canine skull length is a complex trait. (A) Surface scans of a wolf skull morphed to illustrate the differences between brachycephalic, ancestral, and dolichocephalic skull states of canids are shown. Brachycephalic dog breeds have a shortened rostrum (ros), wide zygomatic arches (za), and a rounded neurocranium (nc). In a dolichocephalic dog, the width of the rostrum and zygomatic arches is reduced, and the rostrum tilts ventrally relative to the neurocranium. (B) GWAS of skull length demonstrates that multiple QTL are highly associated with face length. Each dot represents a single marker (a SNP). The y-axis represents the strength of the association [−log10(P-value)]. The x-axis lists marker location by chromosome. Statistically significant associations that exceed correction for multiple testing are indicated in blue. Figure is adapted with permission (Schoenebeck et al. 2012).

At the other end of the continuum is the dolichocephalic appearance of breeds such as the Saluki, Borzoi, and collie. These dogs tend to have narrow, sometimes elongated, snouts and orbitals that are less forward set. A dolichocephalic morphology is exactly what one would predict based on the relationship between morphology and ecology/hunting behavior of wild canids. Many dolichocephalic breeds such as Afghans and Salukis were originally bred for coursing small prey (Figure 2). Thus it make sense that these sighthounds would have a craniofacial configuration predicted to enhance horizon scanning (Miller and Murphy 1995; McGreevy et al. 2004), as is necessary for spotting prey.

Angulation between the skull base and hard palate also differs substantially between dog breeds. Klinorhynchy, the hallmark downward-pointing snout of bull terriers, is morphologically opposite to the rostrum angle observed in breeds such as the boxer and bullmastiff. These examples of brachycephalic breeds display extreme airorhynchy, meaning that their rostra angle dorsally. Thus, the rostrum angle of the bull terrier and the boxer represent opposing extremes of an interbreed continuum (Figure 3A; Nussbaumer 1982).

Figure 3 
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 3 

Craniofacial diversity exists between and within breed dogs. White strips highlight the palate (left) and brainstem (right) in each skull example. (A) The continuum of airorhynchic and klinorhynchic dog breeds, arranged in order of severity. Examples include a Pekingese (1), French bulldog (2), Chow Chow (3), Bernese Mountain Dog (4), German Shepherd (5), and Borzoi (6). (B) Bull terrier skulls demonstrate the continual morphological evolution in breed dogs. Skulls are arranged chronologically from the oldest (top) to the most modern (bottom). Figure is adapted with permission (Nussbaumer 1982).

The neurocranium also bears discussion. Many small dogs from toy and teacup breeds feature brain cases reminiscent of human hydrocephalus. An extreme example is the Chihuahua, whose American Kennel Club breed standard describes the desirable skull as having an “apple dome” and also as permitting open fontanelles—holes within the cranium due to incomplete closure of the skull’s sutures (American Kennel Club 2006). The neuroanatomy of brachycephalic dogs is also quite unusual as the brain is rotated with respect to its mediolateral axis (Roberts et al. 2010). Rotation of the brain in these breeds raises a question about cause and effect: Does the rotation of the brain influence cranial vault shape or vice versa?

Using geometric morphometric analysis, Drake and Klingenberg (2010) found evidence of modularity between the rostrum and the neurocranium of dogs, such that changes in rostrum shape are not strictly correlated with shape changes in the neurocranium. This observation raises the possibility that mixing and matching genetic variants that independently regulate development of either structure enrich canine skull diversity. In mammals and avians, the rostrum and neurocranium are primarily derived from neural crest and paraxial mesoderm, respectively (Noden and Trainor 2005). It is tempting to speculate that canine skull modularity was achieved through selection of genetic variants whose effects alter the development of one or both bone-forming tissues types (Nussbaumer 1976; Noden and Trainor 2005).

Candidate Genes for Skull Variation

Although the morphological variation of canine skulls has been described extensively, the identification of underlying causal genetic variants has only recently become possible. To date, most efforts to identify the genetic underpinnings of canine skull shape have focused on brachycephaly. Early efforts at finding such causal variants have focused on candidates identified in humans.

Human brachycephaly is associated with morbidity and is diagnostic of many syndromes, including Apert’s, Crouzon’s, and Pfeiffer’s syndromes (Miraoui and Marie 2010; Johnson and Wilkie 2011; Ursitti et al. 2011; Levi et al. 2012). In humans, brachycephaly occurs as a result of growth zone defects within the developing skull. Both premature fusion of the coronal suture (bilateral coronal synostosis) and defective endochondral ossification at the synchondroses of the skull base are contributors of brachycephaly (Cendekiawan et al. 2010).

Mutations that affect fibroblast growth factor (Fgf) signaling through compromised receptor activity feature prominently in brachycephalic-type craniosynostosis, as do mutations in genes encoding muscle segment homeobox2 (MSX2) and twist homolog 1 (TWIST1) transcription factors. Other genetic defects leading to craniosynostosis implicate ephrin-B1 (EFNB1), the ras-related protein RAB23, fibrillulin1 (FNB1), P450 (cytochrome), oxidorectortase (POR), transforming growth factor, beta receptor 1 (TGFBR1), and transforming growth factor, beta receptor 2 (TGFBR2). Extrapolating from this short list of candidates, it is clear that FGF and TGF-β signaling are integral for maintaining patency and growth at the sutures and synchondroses (supporting information, Table S1; Online Mendelian Inheritance in Man 2012).

Frontonasal dysplasias can adversely affect face shape in a manner consistent with canine brachycephaly. For example, mutation of Treacher Collins-Franschetti syndrome 1 (TCOF1), whose product normally facilitates ribosome production, results in hypoplasia of frontal and zygoma bones (Dixon et al. 2004; Valdez et al. 2004).

In dogs of all breeds, coronal synostosis is normally absent. However, the cranial base of brachycephalic breeds, as well as some dolichocephalic breeds, is disproportionate to overall body size (Stockard 1941; Lüps 1974). This suggests that regulation of growth at the synchondroses plays a role in the genesis of brachycephalic and dolichocephalic skull conformations.

Taking inspiration from human studies, Haworth et al. (2001) examined the coding sequence from TCOF1 and MSX2 in select dog breeds of varying head shapes. The authors reported a variant at the locus TCOF1 that appeared to be correlated with head shape; however, interpretation of this finding was quickly disputed when additional breeds were examined (Hunemeier et al. 2009). Variation at FGFR3 was examined for its role in canine chondrodysplasia; however, no variants were found when sequences were compared to the Boxer (a brachycephalic breed) reference genome (Smith et al. 2008).

Genome-Wide Association Studies for Finding Loci of Interest

The reference assembly of the dog genome and development of single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) chips enabled geneticists to undertake mapping studies of all types in the dog. Because linkage disequilibrium (LD) is extensive in dogs (Sutter et al. 2004; Lindblad-Toh et al. 2005), with alternating stretches of near homozygosity separated by regions of high heterozygosity, comparably fewer SNPs should be needed to identify associative loci in dogs than in humans, where high levels of heterozygosity are the norm. We and others, using just 100 unrelated cases and controls, showed that only 30,000 informative SNPs are needed to fully interrogate a single trait in the 2.4-Gb dog genome to achieve a 99% probability of successfully detecting an association (Sutter et al. 2004; Lindblad-Toh et al. 2005). In practice, the strong LD within dog breeds is a double-edged sword: GWAS are quite successful at finding genetic associations; however, the resulting critical intervals are often extensive LD blocks that can extend hundreds of kilobases or more. This makes it difficult to go from marker to variant of interest.

For a typical binary GWAS (e.g., a case-control study), the allele frequency differences for one group with a trait are compared to another group without it. In its most basic form, allele frequencies are tested for statistically significant differences using a chi-square test on a marker-by-marker basis. For complex traits such as skull shape, the statistical power of GWAS decreases relative to binary studies. To compensate, we and others have utilized study designs that include multiple breeds with the same trait, which likely share a common ancestral mutation (Goldstein et al. 2006; Parker et al. 2007; Karlsson and Lindblad-Toh 2008). Aside from boosting statistical power, crossbreed comparisons benefit fine mapping, as interbreed-associated haplotypes are inevitably smaller than intrabreed-associated haplotypes.

GWAS test population-based allele frequencies for association to traits of interest. Used in conjunction with selective sweep mapping, it has been possible to detect breeder-selected genetic variation. Based on this approach, Bannasch et al. (2010) used a binary design in which SNP chip genotypes of dogs from nine brachycephalic breeds were compared to nine other breeds. This comparison resulted in detection of a large association on canine chromosome (CFA) 1, as well as a number of other associations that were not described further. Fine mapping on CFA1 defined a 296-kb critical interval that included throbospondin2 (THSB2), next to sparc-related modular calcium binding 2 (SMOC2). While both remain potential candidates genes, no causal variants have been reported to date.

We also used GWAS to map canine traits, favoring treatment of skull shape as a quantitative trait (Boyko et al. 2010; Schoenebeck et al. 2012). Both of our studies relied on the CanMap dataset that entails ∼62,000 SNP profiles from 915 dogs, representing ∼80 breeds. However, the composition of genetic profiles and craniometric data used between the two studies differed. Skull traits profiled by Boyko et al. (2010) were based on linear measurements, while Schoenebeck et al. (2012) used geometric morphometry to quantify nonallometric skull shape. This latter approach demonstrated that principal component 1 (PC1) explained ∼76% of skull variance among skulls that were measured and described the continuum of morphological changes extending between brachycephalic and dolichocephalic dog breeds. Given the difficulties in collecting dog skull data, both studies made an assumption that is arguably unique to dog population studies; given their morphological standardization, “breed average” metrics were used as quantitative traits.

We found an association on CFA1 (Schoenebeck et al. 2012), matching that previously reported (Bannasch et al. 2010; Boyko et al. 2010). Just as important, numerous other associations with snout length and cranium shape were described, including QTL on CFA -5, -24, -30, and -32 and the X chromosome, as well as other QTL that were unique to each study’s design (Figure 2B). These findings begin to explain the complex genetic nature of brachycephaly that was originally predicted by Stockard (1941), as well as shed light on other craniofacial traits.

We fine-mapped the CFA32 QTL, resulting in identification of a phenyalanine → leucine mutation at a highly conserved position within the mature domain of bone morphogenetic protein 3 (BMP3) (Schoenebeck et al. 2012). The biological function of BMP3 is not well understood; however, a number of studies suggest that it can inhibit TGF-β signaling and that it restricts osteogenesis (Bahamonde and Lyons 2001). Postnatal expression in rats indicates that Bmp3 is highly expressed at synchondroses, suggesting a role in chondrogenesis (Kettunen et al. 2006).

In dogs, the BMP3 mutation was nearly fixed in small and medium brachycephalic breeds and was found among a number of smaller breed dogs whose rostrum length tends toward being brachycephalic. Reiterating the genetic complexity of brachycephaly, the mutation was absent from medium-to-large brachycephalic breeds including the boxer (medium), bullmastiff (giant), and Dogue de Bordeaux (giant). As one would expect, the BMP3 mutation was absent from dolichocephalic breeds, with one exception. Curiously, Scottish terriers were also fixed for the mutation, despite their dolichocephalic skull conformation. Whether the effects of the BMP3 mutation manifest permissively in the context of other genetic variation, another variant acts epistatically in Scottish terriers, or Scottish terriers actually do share some aspect of craniofacial shape that is similar to small brachycephalic breeds remains a topic for future investigation. A potentially telling clue is the observation that other asymmetrically chondrodysplastic (short-legged) breeds like Scottish terriers are also carriers of the BMP3 mutation, although the allele frequency of the mutation among these breeds was found to be unfixed (Schoenebeck et al. 2012). This raises the possibility that the effects of the BMP3 mutation extend beyond the skull. Unraveling such anomalies will clearly require the identities and characterization of the causal variation underlying the remaining skull shape QTL, including that on CFA1 described by all three GWAS, as well as consideration of postcranial skeletal traits (Bannasch et al. 2010; Boyko et al. 2010; Schoenebeck et al. 2012).

Finding Additional Skull-Associated Genetic Variants

The associated genetic variation identified in the aforementioned GWAS is “old” since carrier breeds implicitly shared a common founder sometime in their history. Adding to breed relatedness are the remnants of genetic bottlenecks and the use of popular sires among breeders, resulting in genetic substructure and allele frequencies that often deviate from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium. Such confounders make GWAS in dogs prone to generating false-positive associations. Permutation, population stratification, multiple test correction, and the use of mixed models (Kang et al. 2008; Zhang et al. 2010; Lipka et al. 2012; Zhou and Stephens 2012), which take into account genetic potential, fixed effects, and kinship, are essential tools for reducing incidence of false positives that are encountered on a genome-wide scale.

Fine mapping is also fraught with challenges. While comparing multiple breeds helps reduce the extent of LD at a locus of interest, typically the resulting critical interval is still too large and prohibitively expensive to exhaustively Sanger sequence. However, as the cost of targeted sequence capture and next-generation sequencing continue to decrease, fine-mapping regions of extensive LD should become less problematic, enabling unprecedented haplotype resolution and prioritization of genetic variation for further investigation.

While the extensive LD, relatedness, and cryptic genetic structure are potential confounders in nearly all dog GWAS studies, mapping skull traits is uniquely complicated phenotypically, largely because of interbreed differences in scale. Disentangling morphologically allometric variation (size-related) from nonallometric variation is a formidable challenge. Many skulls available for morphometric analysis lack postcranial skeletons, necessitating animal size estimation from the skull itself. Popular proxies of overall size such as the skull’s centroid or cranial base length do not linearly correlate with size across all breeds. There is particular deviation among extreme brachycephalic, dolichocephalic, and some chondrodysplastic breeds (Lüps 1974; Nussbaumer 1976). In practical terms, cross-contamination by allometric variation can be minimized by regressing shape by size during skull quantification and, later, by including a size covariate in the linear regression used for the GWAS.

It is also important to note that morphometric approaches to quantifying shape variation have a strong bearing on what skull traits can be QTL-mapped. Geometric morphometrics offer a three-dimensional perspective of shape variation and are suitable for quantifying robust canine skull shapes such as brachycephaly and dolichocephaly. Other phenotypes such as rostrum angle are poorly captured by such methods because landmark data are typically rotated to determine best fit prior to shape analysis, which effectively removes palate–cranial base angle variation. Principal components analysis (PCA), which is commonly used to categorize shape across fitted datasets, also has shortcomings. As previously noted, PCA tends to bury subtle dog phenotypes by spreading variation across components and can lump similar types of variation together within the same component (Chase et al. 2002; Fondon and Garner 2007). Thus, the shape diversity of dog skulls is probably best described using morphometric approaches that are contextually appropriate.

To date, our skull trait GWAS has relied on use of breed averages as quantitative traits. This approach was done out of necessity, as access to skull data from live, healthy subjects is expensive, scarce, and stressful on the dogs being measured. The breed average skull traits used in our studies were obtained predominantly from museum specimens within the United States and Europe. An unavoidable consequence of this approach is that direct phenotype–genotype relationships are broken. In addition, for traits like brachycephaly, we are unable to determine what percentage of the trait is accounted for by the loci that we discovered. Moreover, for the QTL that we do find, we have no way to determine the rank order of their contribution to the trait. As museums continue to add DNA preservation as a facet of their repositories and as sequencing technologies continue to improve, it is possible that dog morphology association studies using direct phenotype–genotype relationships will be possible. Even with direct phenotype–genotype data, modeling genetic effects could also benefit from outbred populations such as mixed-breed or village dogs (Boyko et al. 2010) to isolate the effects of individual QTL.

The most daunting hurdle of any genetic mapping study is proving genetic causality. For obvious reasons, testing putatively causal variants identified in dogs requires biological surrogate(s), making the effort that much more challenging. Currently, the diversity of commercially available canine cell lines for in vitro studies is almost negligible, lacking chondroblasts and osteoblasts that are ideal for characterizing variants that affect skull shape.

Animal models such as mice and zebrafish can provide means of testing variant functionality though transgenesis, exogenous overexpression, and knockdown. As one might expect, each model has its strengths and disadvantages. In addition to targeted transgenics, the similarity in mammalian genomic architecture makes interrogation of canine intergenic variation possible using mice. Also, owing to their more recent common ancestry, mice are arguably better suited for modeling the mechanistic impact that genetic variants exert on craniofacial morphology. By comparison, zebrafish offer rapid and accessible development and are amenable to gene overexpression, morpholino-mediated knockdown, and certain types of transgenic approaches. Despite interspecific differences in the mammalian and teleost head structures, the genetic pathways that regulate vertebrate craniofacial development are highly conserved across both species (Schilling 1997; Szabo-Rogers et al. 2010). When faced with needing to rapidly evaluate BMP3 function, we turned to zebrafish. Our experiments revealed that Bmp3 plays an ancient role in craniofacial development, and overexpression indicated that functional differences were encoded in the variants that we identified in dogs (Schoenebeck et al. 2012).

Beyond understanding the mechanisms of dog craniofacial diversity, identification of causal genetics is necessary for understanding just what makes dog breeds so morphologically diverse in the first place. Future studies will need to address the origins of the genetic variation that underlies traits like brachycephaly to determine whether variants sprung forth following domestication or were consolidated from wild canids (Wayne and Vonholdt 2012). Also, despite genetic isolation from one another, skull shape continues to rapidly change within many breeds (Figure 3B). Previous studies suggested that hypermutable tandem repeats or SINE element activity could be drivers of dogs’ continuously evolving morphology; however, uncertainty as to what extent such mechanisms actually contribute to morphological diversity remains (Fondon and Garner 2004; Wang and Kirkness 2005; Cordaux and Batzer 2006; Fondon and Garner 2007; Laidlaw et al. 2007). The ideas of Fondon and Garner (2004) are particularly interesting. They examined 37 tandem repeats located within coding regions of developmentally relevant transcription factors in 142 dogs from 92 breeds. Runt-related transcription factor 2 (RUNX2), which is considered to be a master regulator of osteoblast differentiation, was shown to have a modest correlation between its total allele length and alanine/glutamine ratio vs. physical traits such as dorsoventral rostrum bend and midface length (Fondon and Garner 2004). They note that inactivation of RUNX2 causes human cleidocranial cysplasia. Moreover, the authors suggest that slippage events that result in contraction and expansion of tandem repeats represent a novel mechanism of rapid evolution. Regardless of how continual morphological changes occur at the molecular level, mapping nascent causal variation will require new approaches. Whole-genome sequencing may provide the answer, although the analysis of such data is still enormously challenging and arguably less well suited to detect structural variants such as copy number variants and tandem repeats. As we delve deeper into the mysteries of skull morphology, it becomes increasingly apparent that complex and rare genetics are at play in dogs and defining their contributions will be far from routine.

Conclusion

The dog model is young in human years, yet the remarkable insights gleaned from the eight years since its genome’s public debut make it an old soul. Using household pets in biological research is unorthodox, yet it is this animal’s symbiosis with humans that makes it uniquely suited to address the genetic basis of domestication, evolution, morphology, and disease. The pertinence of answers awaiting canine geneticists, we believe, reaches beyond the dog, as we have demonstrated by our discussion of canine craniofacial biology and genetics.

Acknowledgments

We thank our many colleagues who have collaborated with us as well as worked independently to advance the field canine skeletal genetics. We thank Maud Rimbault and Jonine Figueroa for helpful comments on this manuscript. We thank the many institutions that shared their canine skull collections with us. Finally, we thank the many pet owners, veterinarians, and breeders who have supplied us with DNA and information about their dogs for this and other studies. E.A.O. and J.J.S. gratefully acknowledge support from the Intramural Program of the National Human Genome Research Institute. J.J.S. was also supported by a National Institute of General Medical Sciences Pharmacology Research Associate fellowship.

Footnotes

  • Communicating editor: A. S. Wilkins

  • Copyright © 2013 by the Genetics Society of America

Literature Cited

  1. ↵
    American Kennel Club, 2006 The Complete Dog Book, Ed. 20. John Wiley & Sons, New York.
  2. ↵
    1. Bahamonde M. E.,
    2. Lyons K. M.
    , 2001 BMP3: To be or not to be a BMP. J. Bone Joint Surg. Am. 83: S56–S62.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Bannasch D.,
    2. Young A.,
    3. Myers J.,
    4. Truvé K.,
    5. Dickinson P.,
    6. et al.
    , 2010 Localization of canine brachycephaly using an across breed mapping approach. PLoS ONE 5: e9632.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. ↵
    1. Boyko A. R.,
    2. Quignon P.,
    3. Li L.,
    4. Schoenebeck J. J.,
    5. Degenhardt J. D.,
    6. et al.
    , 2010 A simple genetic architecture underlies morphological variation in dogs. PLoS Biol. 8: e1000451.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Cendekiawan T.,
    2. Wong R. W.,
    3. Rabie A. B. M.
    , 2010 Relationships between cranial base synchondroses and craniofacial development: a review. Open Anat. J. 2: 67–75.
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  6. ↵
    1. Chase K.,
    2. Carrier D. R.,
    3. Adler F. R.,
    4. Jarvik T.,
    5. Ostrander E. A.,
    6. et al.
    , 2002 Genetic basis for systems of skeletal quantitative traits: principal component analysis of the canid skeleton. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99: 9930–9935.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  7. ↵
    1. Coppinger R.,
    2. Coppinger L.
    , 2001 Dogs: A Startling New Understanding of Canine Origin, Behavior, and Evolution, Scribner, New York.
  8. ↵
    1. Coppinger R.,
    2. Schneider R.
    , 1995 Evolution of working dogs, pp. 21–50 in The Domestic Dog. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
  9. ↵
    1. Cordaux R.,
    2. Batzer M. A.
    , 2006 Teaching an old dog new tricks: SINEs of canine genomic diversity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103: 1157–1158.
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
    1. Darwin C.
    , 1902 Charles Darwin Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or, the Preservation of Favored Races in the Struggle for Life, Ed. 6. P. F. Collier, New York.
  10. ↵
    1. Dixon J.,
    2. Ellis I.,
    3. Bottani A.,
    4. Temple K.,
    5. Dixon M. J.
    , 2004 Identification of mutations in TCOF1: use of molecular analysis in the pre- and postnatal diagnosis of Treacher Collins syndrome. Am. J. Med. Genet. 127A: 244–248.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. ↵
    1. Drake A. G.
    , 2011 Dispelling dog dogma: an investigation of heterochrony in dogs using 3D geometric morphometric analysis of skull shape. Evol. Dev. 13: 204–213.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. ↵
    1. Drake A. G.,
    2. Klingenberg C. P.
    , 2010 Large-scale diversification of skull shape in domestic dogs: disparity and modularity. Am. Nat. 175: 289–301.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  13. ↵
    1. Fondon J. W. III.,
    2. Garner H. R.
    , 2004 Molecular origins of rapid and continuous morphological evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101: 18058–18063.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. ↵
    1. Fondon J. W. III.,
    2. Garner H. R. H.
    , 2007 Detection of length-dependent effects of tandem repeat alleles by 3-D geometric decomposition of craniofacial variation. Dev. Genes Evol. 217: 79–85.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  15. ↵
    1. Goldstein O.,
    2. Zangerl B.,
    3. Pearce-Kelling S.,
    4. Sidjanin D. J.,
    5. Kijas J. W.,
    6. et al.
    , 2006 Linkage disequilibrium mapping in domestic dog breeds narrows the progressive rod-cone degeneration interval and identifies ancestral disease-transmitting chromosome. Genomics 88: 541–550.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  16. ↵
    1. Gould S. J.
    , 1977 Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Belknap Press, Cambridge, MA.
  17. ↵
    1. Guyon R.,
    2. Lorentzen T. D.,
    3. Hitte C.,
    4. Kim L.,
    5. Cadieu E.,
    6. et al.
    , 2003 A 1-Mb resolution radiation hybrid map of the canine genome. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100: 5296–5301.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. Haworth K. E.,
    2. Islam I.,
    3. Breen M.,
    4. Putt W.,
    5. Makrinou E.,
    6. et al.
    , 2001 Canine TCOF1: cloning, chromosome assignment and genetic analysis in dogs with different head types. Mamm. Genome 12: 622–629.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  19. ↵
    1. Huber W.
    , 1974 Biometric analysis of brachycephaly in domestic dogs. Ann. Biol. 13: 3–4 (in German).
    OpenUrl
  20. ↵
    1. Hunemeier T.,
    2. Salzano F. M.,
    3. Bortolini M. C.
    , 2009 TCOF1 T/Ser variant and brachycephaly in dogs. Anim. Genet. 40: 357–358.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  21. ↵
    1. Johnson D.,
    2. Wilkie A. O.
    , 2011 Craniosynostosis. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 19: 369–376.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. ↵
    1. Kang H. M.,
    2. Zaitlen N. A.,
    3. Wade C. M.,
    4. Kirby A.,
    5. Heckerman D.,
    6. et al.
    , 2008 Efficient control of population structure in model organism association mapping. Genetics 178: 1709–1723.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  23. ↵
    1. Karlsson E. K.,
    2. Lindblad-Toh K.
    , 2008 Leader of the pack: gene mapping in dogs and other model organisms. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9: 713–725.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  24. ↵
    1. Kettunen P.,
    2. Nie X.,
    3. Kvinnsland I. H.,
    4. Luukko K.
    , 2006 Histological development and dynamic expression of Bmp2–6 mRNAs in the embryonic and postnatal mouse cranial base. Anat. Rec. 288: 1250–1258.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  25. ↵
    1. Laidlaw J.,
    2. Gelfand Y.,
    3. Ng K. W.,
    4. Garner H. R.,
    5. Ranganathan R.,
    6. et al.
    , 2007 Elevated basal slippage mutation rates among the Canidae. J. Hered. 98: 452–460.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  26. ↵
    1. Larson G.,
    2. Karlsson E. K.,
    3. Perri A.,
    4. Webster M. T.,
    5. Ho S. Y. W.,
    6. et al.
    , 2012 Rethinking dog domestication by integrating genetics, archeology, and biogeography. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109: 8878–8883.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  27. ↵
    1. Levi B.,
    2. Wan D. C.,
    3. Wong V. W.,
    4. Nelson E.,
    5. Hyun J.,
    6. et al.
    , 2012 Cranial suture biology: from pathways to patient care. J. Craniofac. Surg. 23: 13–19.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    1. Lindblad-Toh K.,
    2. Wade C. M.,
    3. Mikkelsen T. S.,
    4. Karlsson E. K.,
    5. Jaffe D. B.
    , 2005 Genome sequence, comparative analysis and haplotype structure of the domestic dog. Nature 438: 803–819.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  29. ↵
    1. Lipka A. E.,
    2. Tian F.,
    3. Wang Q.,
    4. Peiffer J.,
    5. Li M.,
    6. et al.
    , 2012 GAPIT: Genome Association and Prediction Integrated Tool. Bioinformatics 28: 2397–2399.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. ↵
    1. Lüps P.
    , 1974 Biometric analysis of the skull base of domestic dogs. Zool. Anz. 192: 383–413 (in German).
    OpenUrl
  31. ↵
    1. McGreevy P.,
    2. Grassi T. D.,
    3. Harman A. M.
    , 2004 A strong correlation exists between the distribution of retinal ganglion cells and nose length in the dog. Brain Behav. Evol. 63: 13–22.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  32. ↵
    1. Mellersh C. S.,
    2. Hitte C.,
    3. Richman M.,
    4. Vignaux F.,
    5. Priat C.,
    6. et al.
    , 2000 An integrated linkage-radiation hybrid map of the canine genome. Mamm. Genome 11: 120–130.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  33. ↵
    1. Miller P. E.,
    2. Murphy C. J.
    , 1995 Vision in dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 207: 1623–1634.
    OpenUrlPubMedWeb of Science
  34. ↵
    1. Miraoui H.,
    2. Marie P. J.
    , 2010 Fibroblast growth factor receptor signaling crosstalk in skeletogenesis. Sci. Signal. 3: re9.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  35. ↵
    1. Napierala H.,
    2. Uerpmann H.-P.
    , 2010 A “new” palaeolithic dog from central Europe. Int. J. Osteoarchaeol. 22: 127–137.
    OpenUrl
  36. ↵
    1. Noden D. M.,
    2. Trainor P. A.
    , 2005 Relations and interactions between cranial mesoderm and neural crest populations. J. Anat. 207: 575–601.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  37. ↵
    1. Nussbaumer M.
    , 1976 The problem of the posterior skull base (Hirnstammbasis) in Dachshund skulls. Jb. Naturhist. Mus. Bern 6(1975–1977): 207–220 (in German).
    OpenUrl
  38. ↵
    1. Nussbaumer M.
    , 1978 Biometric analysis of the skull base in small and medium sized dogs. Z. Tierzüchtg. Züchtungsbiol. 95: 1–14 (in German).
    OpenUrl
  39. ↵
    1. Nussbaumer M.
    , 1982 On the variability of dorso-basal curvatures in skulls of domestic dogs. Zool. Anz. 209: 1–32 (in German).
    OpenUrl
  40. ↵
    Online Mendelian Inheritance in Man, OMIM® McKusick-Nathans Institute of Genetic Medicine, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, c1966. Available at: http://omim.org/. Accessed: November 5, 2012.
  41. ↵
    1. Ostrander E. A.
    , 2012 Both ends of the leash: the human links to good dogs with bad genes. N. Engl. J. Med. 367: 636–646.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  42. ↵
    1. Parker H. G.,
    2. Kukekova A. V.,
    3. Akey D. T.,
    4. Goldstein O.,
    5. Kirkness E. F.,
    6. et al.
    , 2007 Breed relationships facilitate fine-mapping studies: a 7.8-kb deletion cosegregates with Collie eye anomaly across multiple dog breeds. Genome Res. 17: 1562–1571.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  43. ↵
    1. Roberts T.,
    2. McGreevy P.,
    3. Valenzuela M.
    , 2010 Human induced rotation and reorganization of the brain of domestic dogs. PLoS ONE 5: e11946.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  44. ↵
    1. Sablin M. V.,
    2. Khlopachev G. A.
    , 2002 The earliest Ice Age dogs: evidence from Eliseevichi 1. Curr. Anthropol. 43: 795–799.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  45. ↵
    1. Schilling T. F.
    , 1997 Genetic analysis of craniofacial development in the vertebrate embryo. Bioessays 19: 459–468.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  46. ↵
    1. Schoenebeck J. J.,
    2. Hutchinson S. A.,
    3. Byers A.,
    4. Beale H. C.,
    5. Carrington B.,
    6. et al.
    , 2012 Variation of BMP3 contributes to dog breed skull diversity. PLoS Genet. 8: e1002849.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  47. ↵
    1. Shearin A. L.,
    2. Ostrander E. A.
    , 2010 Canine morphology: hunting for genes and tracking mutations. PLoS Biol. 8: e1000310.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  48. ↵
    1. Smith L. B.,
    2. Bannasch D. L.,
    3. Young A. E.,
    4. Grossman D. I.,
    5. Belanger J. M.,
    6. et al.
    , 2008 Canine fibroblast growth factor receptor 3 sequence is conserved across dogs of divergent skeletal size. BMC Genet. 9: 67.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  49. ↵
    1. Stockard C. R.
    , 1941 The Genetic and Endocrinic Basis for Differences in Form and Behavior. Wistar Institute of Anatomy and Biology, Philadelphia.
  50. ↵
    1. Sutter N. B.,
    2. Eberle M. A.,
    3. Parker H. G.,
    4. Pullar B. J.,
    5. Kirkness E. F.,
    6. et al.
    , 2004 Extensive and breed-specific linkage disequilibrium in Canis familiaris. Genome Res. 14: 2388–2396.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  51. ↵
    1. Szabo-Rogers H. L.,
    2. Smithers L. E.,
    3. Yakob W.,
    4. Liu K. J.
    , 2010 New directions in craniofacial morphogenesis. Dev. Biol. 341: 84–94.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  52. ↵
    1. Trut L. N.
    , 1999 Early canid domestication: the Farm-Fox Experiment: foxes bred for tamability in a 40-year experiment exhibit remarkable transformations that suggest an interplay between behavioral genetics and development. Am. Sci. 87: 160–169.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  53. ↵
    1. Ursitti F.,
    2. Fadda T.,
    3. Papetti L.,
    4. Pagnoni M.,
    5. Nicita F.,
    6. et al.
    , 2011 Evaluation and management of nonsyndromic craniosynostosis. Acta Paediatr. 100: 1185–1194.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  54. ↵
    1. Valdez B. C.,
    2. Henning D.,
    3. So R. B.,
    4. Dixon J.,
    5. Dixon M. J.
    , 2004 The Treacher Collins syndrome (TCOF1) gene product is involved in ribosomal DNA gene transcription by interacting with upstream binding factor. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 101: 10709–10714.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  55. ↵
    1. Vilà C. C.,
    2. Savolainen P. P.,
    3. Maldonado J. E. J.,
    4. Amorim I. R. I.,
    5. Rice J. E. J.,
    6. et al.
    , 1997 Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. Science 276: 1687–1689.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  56. ↵
    1. Wang W.,
    2. Kirkness E. F.
    , 2005 Short interspersed elements (SINEs) are a major source of canine genomic diversity. Genome Res. 15: 1798–1808.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  57. ↵
    1. Wayne R.
    , 1986 Cranial morphology of domestic and wild canids: the influence of development on morphological change. Evolution 40: 243–261.
    OpenUrlCrossRefWeb of Science
  58. ↵
    1. Wayne R. K.,
    2. vonHoldt B. M.
    , 2012 Evolutionary genomics of dog domestication. Mamm. Genome 23: 3–18.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  59. ↵
    1. Wilcox B.,
    2. Walkowicz C.
    , 1995 Atlas of Dog Breeds of the World, Ed. 5. Tfh Publications, Neptune City, NJ.
  60. ↵
    1. Zeder M. A.
    , 2012 Pathways to animal domestication, pp. 227–259 in Biodiversity in Agriculture, edited by P. Gepts, T. R. Famula, R. L. Bettinger, S. B. Brush, A. B. Damania et al. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.
  61. ↵
    1. Zhang Z.,
    2. Ersoz E.,
    3. Lai C.-Q.,
    4. Todhunter R. J.,
    5. Tiwari H. K.,
    6. et al.
    , 2010 Mixed linear model approach adapted for genome-wide association studies. Nat. Genet. 42: 355–360.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMedWeb of Science
  62. ↵
    1. Zhou X.,
    2. Stephens M.
    , 2012 Genome-wide efficient mixed-model analysis for association studies. Nat. Genet. 44: 821–824.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
View Abstract
Previous ArticleNext Article
Back to top

PUBLICATION INFORMATION

Volume 193 Issue 2, February 2013

Genetics: 193 (2)

ARTICLE CLASSIFICATION

Perspectives
View this article with LENS
Email

Thank you for sharing this Genetics article.

NOTE: We request your email address only to inform the recipient that it was you who recommended this article, and that it is not junk mail. We do not retain these email addresses.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
The Genetics of Canine Skull Shape Variation
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Genetics
(Your Name) thought you would be interested in this article in Genetics.
Print
Alerts
Enter your email below to set up alert notifications for new article, or to manage your existing alerts.
SIGN UP OR SIGN IN WITH YOUR EMAIL
View PDF
Share

The Genetics of Canine Skull Shape Variation

Jeffrey J. Schoenebeck and Elaine A. Ostrander
Genetics February 1, 2013 vol. 193 no. 2 317-325; https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.112.145284
Jeffrey J. Schoenebeck
Cancer Genetics Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Elaine A. Ostrander
Cancer Genetics Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: eostrand@mail.nih.gov
del.icio.us logo Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo CiteULike logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
Citation

The Genetics of Canine Skull Shape Variation

Jeffrey J. Schoenebeck and Elaine A. Ostrander
Genetics February 1, 2013 vol. 193 no. 2 317-325; https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.112.145284
Jeffrey J. Schoenebeck
Cancer Genetics Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Elaine A. Ostrander
Cancer Genetics Branch, National Human Genome Research Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • For correspondence: eostrand@mail.nih.gov

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero

Related Articles

Cited By

More in this TOC Section

  • The Evolution of Science in a Latin-American Country: Genetics and Genomics in Brazil
  • Emerging Properties and Functional Consequences of Noncoding Transcription
  • Mendelism: New Insights from Gregor Mendel’s Lectures in Brno
Show more Perspectives
  • Top
  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Variation in Skull Shape and Dog Domestication
    • Emergence of Dog Breeds
    • Modern Domestic Dog Breed Variation
    • Candidate Genes for Skull Variation
    • Genome-Wide Association Studies for Finding Loci of Interest
    • Finding Additional Skull-Associated Genetic Variants
    • Conclusion
    • Acknowledgments
    • Footnotes
    • Literature Cited
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics

GSA

The Genetics Society of America (GSA), founded in 1931, is the professional membership organization for scientific researchers and educators in the field of genetics. Our members work to advance knowledge in the basic mechanisms of inheritance, from the molecular to the population level.

Online ISSN: 1943-2631

  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Subscribers
  • Submit a Manuscript
  • Editorial Board
  • Press Releases

SPPA Logo

GET CONNECTED

RSS  Subscribe with RSS.

email  Subscribe via email. Sign up to receive alert notifications of new articles.

  • Facebook
  • Twitter
  • YouTube
  • LinkedIn
  • Google Plus

Copyright © 2018 by the Genetics Society of America

  • About GENETICS
  • Terms of use
  • Advertising
  • Permissions
  • Contact us
  • International access